User:Urbourbo/sandbox/moshebar

Most recent edit request to be found on the subject's talk page
Moshe Bar
Moshe Bar
Born
NationalityIsraeli
EducationBen-Gurion University, The Weizmann Institute of Science, University of Southern California and Harvard University
Occupation(s)Cognitive neuroscientist, professor, author

Moshe Bar is an Israeli cognitive neuroscientist. He is a professor at Bar-Ilan University[1] and the Chief Scientific Officer at the Israeli mental health startup Hedonia.[2] He was previously head of the Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center at Bar-Ilan University and before that director of the Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory at Harvard Medical School and Massachusetts General Hospital.[3][4]

Bar’s research focuses on various aspects of brain function, including memory, foresight, mental load, mind-wandering, mood, and creativity. Bar has also contributed to the development of conscious cities, which takes into account the effects of urban design on mental health.[3]

He has published over 80 research articles,[5] contributed several book chapters, and edited two scientific books[6][7].[8] In 2022 he published the popular science book Mindwandering: How Your Constant Mental Drift Can Improve Your Mood and Boost Your Creativity.[9] He received the 2012 Donald O. Hebb Award from the International Neural Network Society,[10] and he is a fellow of the Society of Experimental Psychologists.[11]

Education and professional history edit

Bar completed a bachelor's degree in electrical engineering at Ben-Gurion University in 1988. Thereafter, in parallel with military service in the Israeli Air Force, he completed a master’s degree in computer science and applied mathematics in 1994 at the Weizmann Institute of Science, where he worked under the supervision of Shimon Ullman.[3][4][8]

He pursued doctoral studies in psychology at the University of Southern California, under the supervision of Irving Biederman, earning a Ph.D. in 1998. He continued with postdoctoral research at the psychology department at Harvard University, collaborating with Daniel Schacter and Roger Tootel. Since 2000, Bar held a joint faculty appointment at Harvard Medical School and Massachusetts General Hospital as the director of the Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory before returning to Israel in 2011 to head the Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center at Bar-Ilan University.[3][4][8]

Thereafter, Bar co-founded the Israeli mental health startup company Hedonia,[2] translating the research-based Facilitating Thought Progression (FTP) method, based on his two decades of research on the topic of mood, depression and thought, into mobile games.[12]

Research and scientific contributions edit

Bar has made significant contributions to various areas of study, including visual recognition, predictions in the brain, mental simulations and mindwandering, mood and depression, and aesthetic preference. His work challenges traditional views and sheds light on the complex mechanisms underlying these cognitive processes.

Visual recognition edit

Branching off of his graduate training with professors Shimon Ullman and Irving Biederman, Bar has been using behavioral and neuroimaging (fMRI and MEG) methods to reveal critical aspects of how the brain recognizes objects, scenes, and context in the world around us.[13] This research simultaneously challenged two long-held views. First, together with others, he has argued and shown that the propagation of visual analysis in the cortex is not strictly “bottom-up,” as has been believed for decades, but rather that perception is a result of internally driven top-down processes as much as it is of incoming bottom-up sensory information.[13] As such, his work shows that memory and the prefrontal cortex are active players in visual perception. Bar first proposed this notion in a Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience article 2003,[14] and provided the first significant empirical support in a Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences paper 2006.[13]

A second debate and new domain of research that was steered by Bar’s ideas and studies involves the evasive distinction between spatial and associative processing. Part of the contribution of his research was to characterize the cortical network that mediates the processing of contextual associations, introduced broadly in a Nature Reviews Neuroscience paper 2004.[15] This newly discovered network included a site in the medial temporal cortex that has traditionally been considered as mediating the representation and processing of spatial information.[16][17] His findings offer an alternative interpretation for those previous reports, suggesting that the role of this hippocampal region may more generally involve mediating associative information rather than merely space-related information.[13]

Predictions in the brain edit

Following his research on the role of associations in cognition, Bar’s research has demonstrated the brain's predictive and proactive capabilities.[6] His theoretical and empirical work, focusing on how memory is utilized to generate future predictions, has provided a fresh perspective on cognition and has been a significant part of cognitive neuroscience discourse.[6][18] Bar integrated the various views on predictions in a special issue of the Proceedings of the Royal Society 2009,[19] and then expanded in the book Predictions in the Brain: Using Our Past to Generate a Future edited by Bar for Oxford Press 2011[6].

Mental simulations and mindwandering edit

Bar’s research also provides insight into the human tendency for mental simulations and mindwandering.[20] As suggested by his work, humans use their experiences, stored in memory, to simulate new, imagined experiences, can later be used as predictive scripts that guide our cognition, decisions and action.[19] This concept was first introduced to a broader audience in his 2016 New York Times piece 'Think Less, Think Better'[21], and was later expanded in his book Mindwandering in 2022.[9]

Mood and depression edit

During his time in the US, Bar’s research evolved to include clinical questions, particularly pertaining to psychiatric disorders such as major depression. He started with a theoretical paper in Trends in Cognitive Sciences in 2009[22] that presented a novel synthesis of findings from psychiatry, neuroscience and cognitive psychology, which gave rise to an overarching hypothesis linking mood with thinking patterns and associative processing.[23] The crux of this hypothesis is that a thinking pattern that involves a broad associative scope can elicit positive mood, while a narrow and ruminative thinking pattern can evoke negative mood.[24]

Through collaborations first with the department of psychiatry at the Harvard Medical School and the Massachusetts General Hospital, and since his return to Israel with psychiatric institutions there, this theory has been tested and supported and later implemented as a therapeutic tool, later known as Facilitating Thought Progression (FTP).[24][12]

The behavioral and neuroimaging publications that stemmed from these ideas attracted attention with their global explanatory power and their potential for therapeutic alleviation of symptoms of depression.[25] Bar’s approach, now being employed in healthy and clinical populations,[25] is to train participants with broad associative thinking in a way that will restore their deficient cortical infrastructure and critically diminish ruminative thinking.

Aesthetic preference edit

Bar's research on aesthetics extends into the domains of design, architecture, and public health. His studies on streetscapes, for example, have contributed to design criteria that encourage physical activity.[26] Bar's exploration of the effect of contour on subjective preference and emotion, such as smooth versus sharp, has influenced designers and architects alike.[26]

Select bibliography edit

Scientific publications edit

Books and book chapters edit

Articles to the public edit

Further reading edit

References edit

  1. ^ "Prof. Moshe Bar". Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center (in Hebrew). Retrieved 2023-05-14.
  2. ^ a b "The Hedonia story". Hedonia. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  3. ^ a b c d Blum, Brian (2019-10-06). "Raising the Bar". The Jerusalem Post. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  4. ^ a b c "Prof. Moshe Bar: Adventures in memory". Weizmann Institute. 2016-09-25. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  5. ^ "Moshe Bar". Bar-Ilan University. Retrieved 2023-07-17.
  6. ^ a b c d Bar, Moshe, ed. (2011-06-09). Predictions in the Brain: Using Our Past to Generate a Future. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780195395518.
  7. ^ Kveraga, K.; Bar, M. (2014). Scene Vision: Making Sense of What We See. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.
  8. ^ a b c "Moshe Bar - Director and Professor, Leslie and Susan Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center". World Economic Forum. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  9. ^ a b Sim, Walter (2022-04-30). "Book review: Mind wandering? That may not be a bad thing". The Straits Times. Retrieved 2023-05-14.
  10. ^ "INNS Award Recipients". International Neural Network Society. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  11. ^ "SEP Fellows". Society of Experimental Psychologists. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  12. ^ a b "An entirely new and effective way forward for mental health". Hedonia. Retrieved 2023-05-03.
  13. ^ a b c d Bar, M.; Kassam, K. S.; Ghuman, A. S.; Boshyan, J.; Schmid, A. M.; Schmidt, A. M.; Dale, A. M.; Hämäläinen, M. S.; Marinkovic, K. (2006-01-10). "Top-down facilitation of visual recognition". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 103 (2): 449–454. doi:10.1073/pnas.0507062103. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 1326160. PMID 16407167.
  14. ^ Bar, Moshe (2003-05-15). "A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 15 (4): 600–609. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.296.3039. doi:10.1162/089892903321662976. ISSN 0898-929X. PMID 12803970. S2CID 18209748.
  15. ^ Bar, Moshe (August 2004). "Visual objects in context". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (8): 617–629. doi:10.1038/nrn1476. ISSN 1471-003X. PMID 15263892. S2CID 205499985.
  16. ^ Bar, M.; Aminoff, E. (2003). "Cortical analysis of visual context". Neuron. 38: 347–358.
  17. ^ Aminoff, E.; Gronau, N.; Bar, M. (July 2007). "The Parahippocampal Cortex Mediates Spatial and Nonspatial Associations". Cerebral Cortex. 17: 1493–1503. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl078.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  18. ^ Aminoff, E.M.; Kveraga, K.; Bar, M. (2013). "The role of the parahippocampal cortex in cognition". Trends in Cognitive Science. 17 (8): 379–90.
  19. ^ a b Bar, Moshe (2009-05-12). "Predictions: a universal principle in the operation of the human brain". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1521): 1181–1182. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0321. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 2666718. PMID 19527998.
  20. ^ Bar, M. (2007). "The Proactive Brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (7): 280–289.
  21. ^ Bar, Moshe (June 2016). "Think Less, Think Better". The New York Times.
  22. ^ Bar, Moshe (November 2009). "A cognitive neuroscience hypothesis of mood and depression". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 13 (11): 456–463. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.009. ISSN 1879-307X. PMC 2767460. PMID 19819753.
  23. ^ Herz, Noa; Baror, Shira; Bar, Moshe (2020). "Overarching state of mind". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 24 (3). doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.015.
  24. ^ a b Mason, Malia F.; Bar, Moshe (May 2012). "The effect of mental progression on mood". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 141 (2): 217–221. doi:10.1037/a0025035. ISSN 1939-2222. PMC 3787596. PMID 21823806.
  25. ^ a b Bar, Moshe (2009). "The proactive brain: Memory for predictions". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1521): 1235–1243.
  26. ^ a b Leder, Helmut; Tinio, Pablo P. L.; Bar, Moshe (2011). "Emotional valence modulates the preference for curved objects". Perception. 40 (6): 649–655. doi:10.1068/p6845. ISSN 0301-0066. PMID 21936294. S2CID 36634595.

External links edit