Hunger is a sensation experienced when one feels the physiological need to eat food. In contrast, satiety is the absence of hunger; it is the sensation of feeling full. Appetite is another sensation experienced with eating, however, it differs from hunger; it is the desire to eat food without a physiological need. The often unpleasant feeling of hunger originates from the hypothalamus releasing hormones that target receptors in the liver. A healthy, well-nourished individual can survive for weeks without food intake, with claims ranging from three to ten weeks.[1][2] The sensation of hunger typically manifests after only a few hours without eating and is generally considered to be unpleasant.

Hunger is also the most commonly used term to describe the condition of people who suffer from a chronic lack of sufficient food and constantly or frequently experience the physical sensation of hunger.

Hunger pangs edit

When hunger contractions start to occur in the stomach, they are informally referred to as hunger pangs. Hunger pangs usually do not begin until 12 to 24 hours after the last ingestion of food. A single hunger contraction lasts about 30 seconds, and pangs continue for around 30–45 minutes, then hunger subsides for around 30–150 minutes.[3] Individual contractions are separated at first, but are almost continuous after a certain amount of time.[3] Emotional states (anger, joy etc.) may inhibit hunger contractions.[3] Levels of hunger are increased by lower blood sugar levels, and are higher in diabetics.[3] They reach their greatest intensity in 3 to 4 days and may weaken in the succeeding days[citation needed], although research suggests that hunger never disappears.[4] Hunger contractions are most intense in young, healthy people who have high degrees of gastrointestinal tonus. Periods between contractions increase with old age.[3]

Biological mechanisms edit

The fluctuation of leptin and ghrelin hormone levels results in the motivation of an organism to consume food. When an organism eats, adipocytes trigger the release of leptin into the body. Increasing levels of leptin result in a reduction of one's motivation to eat.[5] After hours of non-consumption, leptin levels drop significantly. These low levels of leptin cause the release of a secondary hormone, ghrelin, which in turn reinitiates the feeling of hunger.

Some studies have suggested that an increased production of ghrelin may enhance appetite evoked by the sight of food, while an increase in stress may also influence the hormone's production.[6] These findings may help to explain why hunger can prevail even in stressful situations.


Short-term regulation of hunger and food intake edit

Short-term regulation of hunger and food intake involves neural signals from the GI tract, blood levels of nutrients, and GI tract hormones.


Neural signals from the GI tract edit

One method that the brain uses to evaluate the contents of the gut is through vagal nerve fibers that carry signals between the brain and the GI tract. Studies have shown that through these vagal nerve fibers, the brain can sense a difference between different macronutrients. Stretch receptors work to inhibit appetite upon distention of the GI tract by sending signals along the vagus nerve afferent pathway and inhibiting the hunger center[7].


Nutrient signals edit

Blood levels of glucose, amino acids, and fatty acids provide a constant flow of information to the brain that may be linked to regulating hunger and energy intake. Nutrient signals that indicate fullness, and therefor inhibit hunger include the following:

  1. Rising blood glucose levels
  2. Elevated blood levels of amino acids
  3. Blood concentrations of fatty acids


Hormone signals edit

The hormones insulin and cholecystokinin (CCK) are released from the GI tract during food absorption and act to suppress feeling of hunger. CCK is key in suppressing hunger because of its role in inhibiting NYP. Glucagon and epinephrin levels rise during fasting and stimulate hunger. Ghrelin, a hormone produced by the stomach, is a powerful hunger stimulant[8].


Long-term regulation of hunger and food intake edit

Leptin, a hormone secreted exclusively by adipose cells in response to an increase in body fat mass, is an important component in the of regulation of long term hunger and food intake. Leptin serves as the brain's indicator of the body's total energy stores. When leptin levels rise in the bloodstream they bind to receptors in ARC.

  1. Suppress the release of NPY, which in turn prevents the release of appetite enhancing orexins from the lateral hypothalamus. This decreases appetite and food intake, promoting weight loss.
  2. Stimulate the expression of CART.

Research has shown that though rising blood levels of leptin do promote weight loss to some extent, it's main role is to protect the body against weight loss in times of nutritional deprivation. Other factors also have been shown to effect long-term hunger and food intake regulation including insulin. [9].

Set-point theories of hunger and eating edit

The set-point theories of hunger and eating are a group of theories developed in the 1940's and 1950's that operate under the assumption that hunger is the result of an energy deficit and that eating is a means by which energy resources are returned to their optimal level, or energy set-point. According to this assumption, a person's energy resources are thought to be at or near their set-point soon after eating, and are thought to decline after that. Once the person's energy levels fall below a certain threshold, the sensation of hunger is experienced, as the body's way of motivating the person to eat again. The set-point assumption is a negative feedback mechanism. [10] Two popular set-point theories include the glucostatic set-point theory and the lipostatic set-point theory.

The set-point theories of hunger and eating present a number of weaknesses. [11]


  1. The current epidemic of obesity and other eating disorders undermines these theories.[12]
  2. The set-point theories of hunger and eating are inconsistent with basic evolutionary pressures related to hunger and eating as they are currently understood.[13]
  3. Major predictions of the set-point theories of hunger and eating have not been confirmed.[14]
  4. They fail to recognize other psychological and social influences on hunger and eating.[15]


Positive-incentive perspective edit

The positive-incentive perspective is an umbrella term for a set of theories presented as an alternative to the set-point theories of hunger and eating. [16] The central assertion to the positive-incentive perspective is the idea that humans and other animals are not normally motivated to eat by energy deficits, but are instead motivated to eat by the anticipated pleasure of eating, or the positive-incentive value.[17] According to this perspective, eating is controlled in much the same way as sexual behavior. Humans engage in sexual behavior, not because of an internal deficit, but instead because they have evolved to crave it. Similarly, the evolutionary pressures of unexpected food shortages have shaped humans and all other warm blooded animals to take advantage of food when it is present. It is the presence of good food, or the mere anticipation of it which makes one hungry. [18]

Premeal hunger edit

Prior to consuming a meal, the body's energy reserves are in reasonable homeostatic balance; however, when a meal is consumed, there is a homeostasis-disturbing influx of fuels into the bloodstream. When the usual mealtime approaches the body takes steps to soften the impact of the homeostasis-disturbing influx of fuels by releasing insulin into the blood, and lowering the blood glucose levels. It is this lowering of blood glucose levels that causes premeal hunger, and not necessarily an energy deficit.[19][20] [21]

Behavioral response edit

Hunger appears to increase activity and movement in many animals - for example, an experiment on spiders showed increased activity and predation in starved spiders, resulting in larger weight gain.[22] This pattern is seen in many animals, including humans while sleeping.[23] It even occurs in rats with their cerebral cortex or stomachs completely removed.[24] Increased activity on hamster wheels occurred when rats were deprived not only of food, but also water or B vitamins such as thiamine.[25] This response may increase the animal's chance of finding food, though it has also been speculated the reaction relieves pressure on the home population.[23] Studies have also shown a difference between the neurological responses human males and females in response to hunger and satiety [26] [27]

Similar cravings edit

A food craving is an intense desire to consume a specific food, as opposed to general hunger. Similarly, thirst is the craving for fluids. Withdrawal craving is a "hunger" for administering addictive drugs.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Ravilious, Kate (2005-12-27). "How long can someone survive without water?". The Guardian. London. Retrieved 2007-05-14.
  2. ^ Lieberson (MD), Alan. "How long can a person survive without food?". Scientific American. Retrieved 12 November 2012.
  3. ^ a b c d e A. J. Carlson (1931) Hunger The Scientific Monthly 33:77-79.
  4. ^ Carlson, A. J. & Hoelzel, F. (1952). The alleged disappearance of hunger during starvation. Science, 115:526-527.
  5. ^ OMIM - LEPTIN; LEP
  6. ^ Malik, S (2007). "Ghrelin modulates brain activity in areas that control appetitive behavior". Cell Metabolism. 7 (5): 400–9. doi:10.1016/j.cmet.2008.03.007. PMID 18460331. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  7. ^ Marieb, E., & Marieb, E. (2010). Human anatomy & physiology. (8th ed. ed., pp. 945-947). San Fransisco: Pearson Benjamin Cummings.
  8. ^ Marieb, E., & Marieb, E. (2010). Human anatomy & physiology. (8th ed. ed., pp. 945-947). San Fransisco: Pearson Benjamin Cummings.
  9. ^ Marieb, E., & Marieb, E. (2010). Human anatomy & physiology. (8th ed. ed., pp. 945-947). San Fransisco: Pearson Benjamin Cummings.
  10. ^ Wenning, A. (1990). Sensing effectors make sense. Trends in Neurosciences, 22, 550-555
  11. ^ de Castro, J.M., & Plunkett, S. (2002). A general model of intake regulation. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 26, 581-595.
  12. ^ Pinel, J. P. J., Biopsychology, 6th ed. 293-294. ISBN 0-205-42651-4
  13. ^ Pinel, J. P. J., Assanand, S., & Lehman, D. R. (2000). Hunger, eating, and ill health. American Psychologist, 55, 1105-1116.
  14. ^ Lowe, M. R. (1993). The effects of dieting on eating and behavior: A three-factor model. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 100-121.
  15. ^ Pinel, J. P. J., Biopsychology, 6th ed. 293-294. ISBN 0-205-42651-4
  16. ^ Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiology and Behavior, 81, 179-209.
  17. ^ Booth, D. A. (1981). The physiology of appetite. British Medical Bulletin, 37, 135-140.
  18. ^ Pinel, J. P. J., Biopsychology, 6th ed. 293-294. ISBN 0-205-42651-4
  19. ^ Woods, S. C. (1991). The eating paradox: How we tolerate food. Psychological Review, 98, 488-505.
  20. ^ Woods, S. C. (2004). Lessons in the interaction of hormones and ingestive behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 82(1), 187-190.
  21. ^ Woods, S. C., & Ramsay, D. S. (2000). Pavlovian influences over food and drug intake. Behavioral Brain Research, 110, 175-182.
  22. ^ Provencher, L. & Riechert, S. E. (1991) Short-Term Effects of Hunger Conditioning on Spider Behavior, Predation, and Gain of Weight Oikos 62:160-166
  23. ^ a b Wald, G.; Jackson, B. (1944) Activity and Nutritional Deprivation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 30:255-263
  24. ^ "George Wald: The Origin of Death". Retrieved 2007-05-14.
  25. ^ Guerrant, N.B., Dutcher, R.A. (1940) Journal of Nutrition 20:589.
  26. ^ Parigi, Del Angelo, et. al. (2001) Sex differences in the human brain's response to hunger and satiation. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition vol. 106 no. 4. pp. 1249–1254.
  27. ^ Gene-Jack Wang, Nora D. Volkow, Frank Telang, Millard Jayne, Yeming Ma, Kith Pradhan, Wei Zhu, Christopher T. Wong, Panayotis K. Thanos, Allan Geliebter, Anat Biegon, and Joanna S. Fowler Evidence of gender differences in the ability to inhibit brain activation elicited by food stimulation. (2009) PNAS 106 (4) 1249-1254; published ahead of print January 21, 2009, doi:10.1073/pnas.0807423106


Category:Food and drink Category:Hunger Category:Motivation Category:Neuropsychology Category:Limbic system