Rumination is the focused attention on the symptoms of one's mental distress. In 1998, Nolen-Hoeksema proposed the Response Styles Theory,[1][2] which is the most widely used conceptualization model of rumination. However, other theories have proposed different definitions for rumination. For example, in the Goal Progress Theory, rumination is conceptualized not as a reaction to a mood state, but as a "response to failure to progress satisfactorily towards a goal".[3] According to multiple studies, rumination is a mechanism that develops and sustains psychopathology conditions such as anxiety, depression, and other negative mental disorders.[4] There are some defined models of rumination, mostly interpreted by the measurement tools. [5] Multiple tools exist to measure ruminative thoughts. Treatments specifically addressing ruminative thought patterns are still in the early stages of development.[6]
Theories
editResponse styles theory
editResponse styles theory (RST) initially defined rumination as passively and repetitively focusing on one's symptoms of depression and the possible causes and consequences of these symptoms.[7] As evidence for this definition, rumination has been implicated in the development, maintenance, and aggravation of both depressive symptoms,[8][9] as well as episodes of major depression.[10] Recently, RST has expanded the definition of rumination beyond depression to include passive and repetitive focus on the causes, consequences, and symptoms of one's distress in general. This change was made because rumination has been implicated in a host of disorders, not just depression.[11][1]
RST also contends that positive distraction is the healthy alternative to rumination, where focus is directed to positive stimuli instead of to distress.[12] However, the literature suggests that positive distraction may not be as potent a tool as once thought.[11][1]
Specifically, the S-REF model defines rumination as "repetitive thoughts generated by attempts to cope with self-discrepancy that are directed primarily toward processing the content of self-referent information and not toward immediate goal-directed action."[13] Put more simply, when a person ruminates, they aim to answer questions such as:
- How do I feel about this event?
- How can I change my thoughts and feelings about the event?
- How can I prevent disturbing thoughts and feelings in the future?
However, in answering these questions, ruminators tend to focus on their emotions (i.e., "self-referent information") as opposed to problem solving (i.e., "goal-directed action").[13]
Meta-cognition is also an important part of the S-REF model and helps to explain the link between rumination and depression.[7] Specifically, those who hold "positive meta-cognitive beliefs" about rumination (to make sense of negative thoughts and emotions or ensure the prevention of the same)[14] are perhaps initially motivated to engage in rumination with high perseverance.[15] However, individuals who have engaged themselves in positive acts of rumination were more likely to use rumination as a coping mechanism upon encountering negative emotions. This causes the individual to modify his/her perception of rumination as unpleasant, unmanageable and "socially damaging" in general.[15][16] Rumination additionally has a tendency to magnify with an up-regulation of emotions in the body, thus beginning the downward-moving spiral of depression.[17] The individual's "negative meta-cognitive" beliefs then contribute to the development and maintenance of depression.[15]
Goal progress theory
editGoal progress theory (GPT), sometimes referred to as Control Theory, seeks to explain rumination as a function of goal progress. Specifically, GPT views rumination as an example of the Zeigarnik Effect, which suggests that individuals are more likely to remember information from unfinished tasks than from finished tasks.[3][11] From this understanding, GPT defines rumination as "the tendency to think recurrently about important, higher order goals that have not yet been attained" or towards which sufficient progress has not been made.[11][18]
GPT predicts that individuals for whom goal-related information is highly accessible should be more likely to ruminate. Various studies have provided support for this prediction.[3] However, the rumination experienced is focused more towards problem solving than rumination described by RST.[19]
Pathology
editExtensive research on the effects of rumination, or the tendency to self-reflect, shows that the negative form of rumination (associated with dysphoria) interferes with people's ability to focus on problem-solving and results in dwelling on negative thoughts about past failures.[20] Evidence from studies suggests that the negative implications of rumination are due to cognitive biases, such as memory and attentional biases, which predispose ruminators to selectively devote attention to negative stimuli.[21]
The organic causes of rumination are not fully understood. Research has identified the activation of certain regions in the brain's default mode networks as neural substrates of rumination, but the number of brain-imaging studies on rumination is limited.[22]
The tendency to negatively ruminate is a stable constant over time and serves as a significant risk factor for clinical depression. Not only are habitual ruminators more likely to become depressed, but experimental studies have demonstrated that people who are induced to ruminate experience greater depressed mood.[8] There is also evidence that rumination is linked to general anxiety, post traumatic stress, binge drinking, eating disorders, and self-injurious behavior.[1] Research suggests that rumination is somewhat associated with a higher frequency of non-suicidal self-injury, and more heavily associated with a history of non-suicidal self injury.[23]
Rumination was originally believed to predict the duration of depressive symptoms. In other words, ruminating about problems was presumed to be a form of memory rehearsal which was believed to actually lengthen the experience of depression. The evidence now suggests that although rumination contributes to depression, it is not necessarily correlated with the duration of symptoms.[1]
Research on the relationships between executive functions and rumination has yielded mixed results. Some studies have observed a negative correlation with two executive functioning abilities, set-shifting and inhibition, but the magnitudes of those relationships are unclear.[24][25] Another study observed only one relationship between rumination and one executive function, specifically the ability to discard past information from working memory.[26] Other studies, however, found no relationship between rumination and working memory.[24][25]
Types
editTheories of rumination differ in their predictions regarding the content of ruminative thoughts based on their respective conceptualizations. Some models propose that rumination is focused on negative feeling states and/or the circumstances surrounding that emotion (RST, rumination on sadness, five-factor model,[27] negative cognitive style,[28] social phobia[29] models). Rumination in other models focuses on discrepancies between one's current and desired status (goal progress, conceptual evaluative model of rumination). Finally, other models propose that it is the negative themes of uncontrollability and harm in metacognitions that are most important.[1] Some common thoughts that are characteristic of ruminative responses are questioning the well-being of oneself and focusing on the possible causes and consequences of one's depressive symptoms (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). For example, some ruminative thoughts include "why am I such a loser", "I'm in such a bad mood" or "I just don't feel like doing anything".[12]
There exist several types of rumination.
- State rumination, which involves dwelling on the consequences and feelings associated with the failure. State rumination is more common in people who are pessimistic, neurotic, and who have negative attributional styles.[30]
- Action rumination, which consists of task-oriented thought processes focused on goal-achievement and correction of mistakes.[30]
- Task-irrelevant rumination, which utilizes events or people unassociated with the blocked goal to distract a person from the failure.[30][31]
- Post-event processing is pervasive detailed reviewing of socially failed or embarrassing activities or situations that subjects perceive even more negatively/anxiety-provoking than they were, and they reinforce with pervasive recollections of similarly biased memories.[29][32]
- Stress-reactive rumination is a pervasive detailed reviewing of painful experiences that specific stressful events induced and a cognitive bias enhances by memorising these events generally more stressful that they were while triggering depression.[28][33]
Measurement
editThere are multiple tools for measuring rumination. These include the following:
Ruminative responses scale
editThe tendency to ruminate can be assessed with the Ruminative Responses Scale of the Response Styles Questionnaire.[34][35] On this measure, people are asked to indicate how often they engage in 22 ruminative thoughts or behaviors when they feel sad or blue.
Rumination on sadness scale
editThe Rumination On Sadness Scale is a self-report tool consisting of 13 items that uses the Likert Scale to measure rumination of sadness.[36]
Repetitive thought/thinking questionnaire
editThe 31-item Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire (RTQ) measures worry, rumination, and post-event processing with the purpose of controlling for effects associated with a psychological diagnosis or disorder. It includes two subscales, Repetitive Negative Thinking (RNT) and Absence of Repetitive Thinking (ART). RNT is associated with anxiety, depression, and other negative emotions as it influences metacognitive beliefs, cognitive avoidance strategies, and maladaptive thought control strategies.[37] The ART subscale reflects the absence of those negative emotions associated with the RNT subscale, essentially measuring their opposites.[38]
Rumination-reflection scale
editThe Rumination-Reflection Scale involves 24 items. Half of the questions look for adaptive reflective thought while the other half note self-rumination focus. This scale incorporates the Likert Scale.[39]
Dynamic assessment of rumination
editRecently, researchers have started to develop a validated measurement protocol to best assess rumination in a dynamic fashion using experience sampling methodology.[40]
Sex differences
editAccording to Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, women tend to ruminate when they are depressed, whereas men tend to distract themselves. This difference in response style was proposed to explain the higher rates of depression in women compared to men.[41] Research has supported the theory that women have a greater likelihood to ruminate than men, but the magnitude of this difference seems to be small.[42] The prediction that men are more likely to distract themselves has not been consistently supported in research.[43]
A meta-analysis was performed on both the sex differences in rumination of adults and the rumination subtypes "brooding" and "reflection." Studies show that women's chances of experiencing depressive symptoms or depression was twice that of men. The response styles theory (RST) suggests this may be due, to some extent, to higher rates of rumination in women. Brooding can be operationalized as continuous, passive, negative internalized thoughts. It is highly connected to worsening depression. Reflection is neutral, rather than negative, more active observation of self. In the meta-analysis, women showed statistically significant increases in levels of both brooding and reflection, supporting RST. Interestingly, there was a much smaller sex difference in reflection than brooding. The meta-analyses found similar results across multiple study designs.[44]
Healthy self-disclosure
editAlthough rumination is generally unhealthy and associated with depression, thinking and talking about one's feelings can be beneficial under the right conditions. According to Pennebaker, healthy self-disclosure can reduce distress and rumination when it leads to greater insight and understanding about the source of one's problems.[45] Thus, when people share their feelings with others in the context of supportive relationships, they are likely to experience growth. In contrast, when people repetitively ruminate and dwell on the same problem without making progress, they are likely to experience depression. Co-rumination is a process defined as "excessively discussing personal problems within a dyadic relationship",[46] a construct that is relatively understudied in both its negative and positive trade-offs.
Treatment
editRumination-focused cognitive behavioral therapy (RFCBT)
editSome studies have begun developing a type of cognitive behavioral therapy that focuses on rumination.[47] Rumination-focused cognitive behavior therapy (RFCBT) aims to teach patients to recognize when they begin to ruminate and ultimately re-frame the way they view themselves.[6] The theories behind RFCBT as a treatment for rumination emphasize the fact that rumination is a destructive habit, and is mostly due to an individual's abstract cognitive processing. [48] The approach that a therapist takes is to discuss with their client to change their thoughts into a healthy style of thinking. Instead clients allowing negative repetitive thoughts to take over their daily life, therapists suggest that they process them into constructive thinking , which are helpful, process-focused, and concrete thoughts.[49] In practice, this can look like the therapist prompting a client to replace their abstract ruminating "why" questions with more concrete "how" questions, that can be more easily examined and answered.[50] Support for these interventions has come from a multitude of studies, suggesting that implementation of both individualized and group RFCBT has been correlated with lower rumination in adolescents and young adults, both with and without major depression or anxiety disorders. [51] [52]
Mindfulness-based intervention (MBI)
editAs mentioned in the lead section, rumination associates with other negative mental health conditions. Depression is one of mental diseases that are cause by genetic, environmental, and mental factors that experience ruminative thoughts.[4] One study implicates how mindfulness-based intervention can decrease the symptoms of rumination. The process of mindfulness based intervention is: (1) the interest in acceptance, (2) the defusion of thoughts and emotions, (3) the importance of being in the present moment, (4) self as context. These mechanisms gives the ability to not suppress or avoid emotions but to encounter them without giving judgment .[53]
Specifically, the process of Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) has been generally correlated with lower rumination symptoms in both patients with various mental disorders and healthy patients.[48] This process includes practices like meditation, body scans, and other nonjudgmental methods, mainly focusing on breath and passing thoughts. These practices can help individuals either let their ruminating thoughts pass or reduce their focus on them, by pulling focus onto things like their breath. [48]
Relationship to other related constructs
editRumination has been confounded with other similar constructs that may overlap with it. Worry, negative automatic thoughts, and avoidance are a few of them.
Worry
editRumination appears closely related to worry. Some models consider rumination to be a type of worry (S-REF).[11] Worry has been identified as "a chain of thoughts and images, negatively affect-laden and relatively uncontrollable; it represents an attempt to engage in mental problem solving on an issue whose outcome is uncertain, but contains the possibility of one or more negative outcomes."[11][54]
Worry is often studied in the context of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), whereas rumination is often studied in the context of major depressive disorder. Because of the high comorbidity of these two conditions, more recent research is exploring the overlap of worry and rumination.[11]
According to the Mental Health Foundation, rumination has been identified to be one of the main problems that leads to anxiety and depression. A study conducted by psychologists from the University of Liverpool suggests that dwelling on negative events that have occurred in one's life is the biggest predictor of depression and anxiety.[55]
Measures of rumination and worry have also demonstrated high correlations, above and beyond that of symptom measures of anxiety and depression (r=.66; Beck & Perkins, 2001).[56] Rumination and worry overlap in their relationships to anxiety and depression, although some studies do indicate specificity of rumination to depression and worry to anxiety. Rumination has been found to predict changes in both depression and anxiety symptoms and individuals with major depression have been reported to engage in levels of worry similar to individuals with GAD. As a whole, these studies suggest that rumination and worry are related not only to each other, but also each is related to symptoms of both depression and anxiety.
Other studies have demonstrated that the content of worry and rumination are distinct; worry thoughts are often focused on problem-solving and have a future orientation, whereas ruminative thoughts concern themes of loss and are more focused on the past. Rumination, as compared to worry, has also been associated with less effort and less confidence in problem solving (Papageorgiou & Wells, 2004). It has also been suggested that rumination and worry serve different purposes, namely that rumination is associated with greater belief in the personal relevance of a situation and a larger need to understand it, whereas worry is associated with a desire to avoid worry thoughts (Watkins 2004b). Worry has also been hypothesized to contain more imagery than rumination; however, support for this has been mixed.[57][58][59]
Overall, these studies suggest that worry and rumination are related constructs that both lead to depression and anxiety. It is likely that rumination and worry, as with rumination and reflection, are related types of repetitive negative thinking that may be better captured as subtypes of some larger construct, such as avoidant coping strategies.
Automatic negative thoughts
editRumination has been compared to automatic negative thoughts, defined as repetitive thoughts that contain themes of personal loss or failure. Nolen-Hoeksema (2004) contends that rumination (as defined in RST) is distinct from negative automatic thoughts in that while negative automatic thoughts are relatively shorthand appraisals of loss and depression in depression, rumination consists of longer chains of repetitive, recyclic, negative and self-focused thinking that may occur as a response to initial negative thoughts.[50] Nolen-Hoeksema also suggests that rumination may, in addition to analysis of symptoms, causes, and consequences, contain negative themes like those in automatic thoughts. Similarly, Papageorgiou and Wells (2004) have provided supports to this conclusion when they found that rumination can predict depression even when negative cognitions are controlled, suggesting that these constructs do not wholly overlap and have different predictive value.[13] Despite Nolen-Hoeksema's (2004) argument that rumination and negative automatic thoughts are distinct phenomena, the Response Style Questionnaire has been criticized for its conceptual overlap with negative automatic thoughts.
Avoidance
editAvoidance can absolutely overlap with the habit of rumination, particularly because rumination is an avoidant action in itself. [48] Simply because the action of actively avoiding certain thoughts is difficult to achieve without ending up in rumination, the vary practice of thought avoidance can actually increase rumination, even though the idea of avoiding thoughts and ruminating on them seem to be in complete opposition.[48] There is evidence to support this idea, with studies supporting the fact that unsuccessful attempts to suppress particular thoughts are significantly related with strong rumination tendencies in some individuals. [60]
Comorbid Disorders
editResearch has determined that rumination can be highly correlated with other psychiatric disorders.[61] [62] Some of these disorders that are related to high rumination behaviors include Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD), Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD), and Anorexia Nervosa.[61] Individuals struggling with these disorders, when compared to individuals with no mental health issues, reported higher rates of rumination.[61] In addition to these, individuals with Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) also experience rumination as a symptom of their disorder.[62] In individuals with OCD, rumination serves as a compulsion, meaning individuals compulsively ruminate over thoughts and worries related to their obsessions.[62]
See also
editReferences
edit- ^ a b c d e f Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S (September 2008). "Rethinking Rumination". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 3 (5): 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x. PMID 26158958. S2CID 6415609.
- ^ Nolan SA, Roberts JE, Gotlib IH (1998). "Neuroticism and ruminative response style as predictors of change in depressive symptomatology" (PDF). Cognitive Therapy and Research. 22 (5): 445–455. doi:10.1023/A:1018769531641. S2CID 15419457.
- ^ a b c Watkins ER (March 2008). "Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought". Psychological Bulletin. 134 (2): 163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163. PMC 2672052. PMID 18298268.
- ^ a b Li, Pan; Mao, Lingyun; Hu, Maorong; Lu, Zihang; Yuan, Xin; Zhang, Yanyan; Hu, Zhizhong (January 2022). "Mindfulness on Rumination in Patients with Depressive Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials". International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 19 (23): 16101. doi:10.3390/ijerph192316101. ISSN 1660-4601. PMC 9737922. PMID 36498174.
- ^ Smith, Jeannette M.; Alloy, Lauren B. (1 March 2009). "A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct". Clinical Psychology Review. 29 (2): 116–128. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003. ISSN 0272-7358. PMC 2832862. PMID 19128864.
- ^ a b Feldhaus, Claudia G.; Jacobs, Rachel H.; Watkins, Edward R.; Peters, Amy T.; Bessette, Katie L.; Langenecker, Scott A. (28 April 2020). "Rumination-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Decreases Anxiety and Increases Behavioral Activation Among Remitted Adolescents". Journal of Child and Family Studies. 29 (7): 1982–1991. doi:10.1007/s10826-020-01711-7. ISSN 1062-1024. PMC 7968389. PMID 33737799.
- ^ a b Papageorgiou C, Wells A (2004). "Nature, Functions, and Beliefs about Depressive Rumination". In Papageorgiou C, Wells A (eds.). Depressive Rumination: Nature, Theory and Treatment. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 1–20. doi:10.1002/9780470713853.ch1. ISBN 9780470713853.
- ^ a b Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J (1993). "Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood". Cognition and Emotion. 7 (6): 561–570. doi:10.1080/02699939308409206.
- ^ Nolen-Hoeksema S, Parker LE, Larson J (July 1994). "Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (1): 92–104. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.92. PMID 8046585.
- ^ Nolen-Hoeksema S (August 2000). "The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 109 (3): 504–511. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.109.3.504. PMID 11016119. S2CID 43090073.
- ^ a b c d e f g Smith JM, Alloy LB (March 2009). "A roadmap to rumination: a review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct". Clinical Psychology Review. 29 (2): 116–128. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003. PMC 2832862. PMID 19128864.
- ^ a b Nolen-Hoeksema S (November 1991). "Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 100 (4): 569–582. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.100.4.569. PMID 1757671.
- ^ a b c Matthews G, Wells A (2004). "Rumination, Depression, and Metacognition: The S-REF Model". In Papageorgiou C, Wells A (eds.). Depressive Rumination: Nature, Theory and Treatment. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 125–151. doi:10.1002/9780470713853.ch7. ISBN 9780470713853.
- ^ Papageorgiou C, Wells A (2001). "Positive beliefs about depressive rumination: Development and preliminary validation of a self-report scale". Behavior Therapy. 32 (1): 13–26. doi:10.1016/s0005-7894(01)80041-1.
- ^ a b c Papageorgiou C, Wells A (2003). "An empirical test of a clinical meta-cognitive model of rumination and depression". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 27 (3): 261–273. doi:10.1023/A:1023962332399. S2CID 23020041.
- ^ Papageorgiou C, Wells A (2001). "Meta-cognitive beliefs about rumination in recurrent major depression". Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 8 (2): 160–164. doi:10.1016/s1077-7229(01)80021-3.
- ^ Nejad AB, Fossati P, Lemogne C (October 2013). "Self-referential processing, rumination, and cortical midline structures in major depression". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 666. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00666. PMC 3794427. PMID 24124416.
- ^ Martin LL, Shrira I, Startup HM (2004). "Rumination as a Function of Goal Progress, Stop Rules, and Cerebral Lateralization". In Papageorgiou C, Wells A (eds.). Depressive Rumination: Nature, Theory and Treatment. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 153–175. doi:10.1002/9780470713853.ch8. ISBN 9780470713853.
- ^ Roberts H, Watkins ER, Wills AJ (December 2013). "Cueing an unresolved personal goal causes persistent ruminative self-focus: an experimental evaluation of control theories of rumination". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 44 (4): 449–455. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.05.004. hdl:10871/33114. PMID 23810947.
- ^ Lyubomirsky S, Kasri F, Zehm K (2003). "Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 27 (3): 309–330. doi:10.1023/A:1023918517378. S2CID 14204781.
- ^ Joormann J, Dkane M, Gotlib IH (September 2006). "Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases" (PDF). Behavior Therapy. 37 (3): 269–280. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.01.002. PMID 16942978. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-06-01.
- ^ Zhou, Hui-Xia; Chen, Xiao; Shen, Yang-Qian; Li, Le; Chen, Ning-Xuan; Zhu, Zhi-Chen; Castellanos, Francisco Xavier; Yan, Chao-Gan (2020). "Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression". NeuroImage. 206: 116287. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116287. ISSN 1053-8119. PMID 31655111. S2CID 204837753.
- ^ Coleman, Sophie E.; Dunlop, Brendan J.; Hartley, Samantha; Taylor, Peter J. (22 November 2021). "The relationship between rumination and NSSI: A systematic review and meta-analysis". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 61 (2): 405–443. doi:10.1111/bjc.12350. ISSN 0144-6657. PMID 34806214. S2CID 244519847.
- ^ a b Yang, Yingkai; Cao, Songfeng; Shields, Grant S.; Teng, Zhaojun; Liu, Yanling (5 July 2016). "The relationships between rumination and core executive functions: A meta-analysis". Depression and Anxiety. 34 (1): 37–50. doi:10.1002/da.22539. ISSN 1091-4269. PMID 27378739. S2CID 27626857.
- ^ a b Vălenaș, Sergiu P.; Szentágotai-Tătar, Aurora (1 September 2017). "The relationship between rumination and executive functions: A meta-analysis". Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies. 17 (2): 23–52. doi:10.24193/jebp.2017.2.2. ISSN 2360-0853.
- ^ Zetsche, Ulrike; Bürkner, Paul-Christian; Schulze, Lars (7 June 2018). "Shedding light on the association between repetitive negative thinking and deficits in cognitive control – a meta-analysis". PsyArXiv. doi:10.31234/osf.io/bz2ck. S2CID 242102055.
Zetsche, Ulrike; Bürkner, Paul-Christian; Schulze, Lars (2018). "Shedding light on the association between repetitive negative thinking and deficits in cognitive control – A meta-analysis". Clinical Psychology Review. 63: 56–65. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.001. ISSN 0272-7358. PMID 29913351. S2CID 49313189. - ^ Trapnell D, Campbell D (1999). "Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76 (2): 284–304. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284. PMID 10074710.
- ^ a b Robinson M, Alloy L (2003). "Negative cognitive styles and stress-reactive rumination interact to predict depression: a prospective study". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 27 (3): 275–291. doi:10.1023/A:1023914416469. S2CID 13358745.
- ^ a b Clark M, Wells A (1995). "A cognitive model of social phobia". In Heimberg R, Liebowitz M, Hope D, Schneier F (eds.). Social phobia: diagnosis, assessment, and treatment. London: Guilford Publications. ISBN 1572300124.
- ^ a b c Mikulincer M (1996). "Mental rumination and learned helplessness: Cognitive shifts during helplessness training and their behavioral consequences.". In Sarason I, Sarason B, Pierce G (eds.). Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 191–210.
- ^ Ciarocco NJ, Vohs KD, Baumeister RF (2010). "Some Good News About Rumination: Task-Focused Thinking After Failure Facilitates Performance Improvement". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 29 (10): 1057–1073. doi:10.1521/jscp.2010.29.10.1057. S2CID 14689415.
- ^ Fehm L, Schneider G, Hoyer J (2007). "Is post-event processing specific for social anxiety?". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 38 (1): 11–22. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.02.004. PMID 16730328.
- ^ Rood L, Roelofs J, Bögels M (2012). "Stress-reactive rumination, negative cognitive style, and stressors in relationship to depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 41 (4): 414–425. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9657-3. PMC 3298737. PMID 21451946. S2CID 254740492.
- ^ Nolen-Hoeksema S (November 1991). "Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 100 (4): 569–582. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569. ISSN 1939-1846. PMID 1757671.
- ^ Bernstein EE, Heeren A, McNally RJ (June 2019). "Reexamining trait rumination as a system of repetitive negative thoughts: A network analysis". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 63: 21–27. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.12.005. PMID 30590225. S2CID 58536838.
- ^ Roelofs J, Muris P, Huibers M, Peeters F, Arntz A (December 2006). "On the measurement of rumination: a psychometric evaluation of the ruminative response scale and the rumination on sadness scale in undergraduates". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 37 (4): 299–313. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.03.002. PMID 16678121....
- ^ Mahoney AE, McEvoy PM, Moulds ML (March 2012). "Psychometric properties of the Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire in a clinical sample". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 26 (2): 359–367. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.003. PMID 22204788.
- ^ McEvoy, Peter M.; Mahoney, Alison E.J.; Moulds, Michelle L. (2010). "Are worry, rumination, and post-event processing one and the same?". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 24 (5): 509–519. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.008. ISSN 0887-6185. PMID 20409676.
- ^ Janssen R (1 February 2021). "The Pleated Dress of Nywty". PalArch's Journal of Archaeology of Egypt / Egyptology. 17 (1): 1–11. doi:10.48080/jae.v17i1.3. ISSN 1567-214X. S2CID 234010733.
- ^ Hoebeke, Yorgo; Blanchard, M Annelise; Contreras, Alba; Heeren, Alexandre (2022). "An Experience Sampling Measure of the Key Features of Rumination". Clinical Neuropsychiatry. 19 (5): 288–297. doi:10.36131/cnfioritieditore20220504. PMC 9597648. PMID 36340270.
- ^ Nolen-Hoeksema S (March 1987). "Sex differences in unipolar depression: evidence and theory". Psychological Bulletin. 101 (2): 259–282. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259. PMID 3562707. S2CID 5026228.
- ^ Johnson, Daniel P.; Whisman, Mark A. (2013). "Gender differences in rumination: A meta-analysis". Personality and Individual Differences. 55 (4): 367–374. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019. ISSN 0191-8869. PMC 3786159. PMID 24089583.
- ^ Strauss J, Muday T, McNall K, Wong M (1997). "Response Style Theory revisited: Gender differences and stereotypes in rumination and distraction". Sex Roles. 36 (11/12): 771–792. doi:10.1023/A:1025679223514. S2CID 142916986.
- ^ Johnson DP, Whisman MA (August 2013). "Gender differences in rumination: A meta-analysis". Personality and Individual Differences. 55 (4): 367–374. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019. PMC 3786159. PMID 24089583.
- ^ Pennebaker JW (1989). "Confession, Inhibition, and Disease". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 22. pp. 211–244. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60309-3. ISBN 9780120152223.
- ^ Rose AJ (2002). "Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys". Child Development. 73 (6): 1830–1843. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00509. PMID 12487497.
- ^ Watkins, Ed; Scott, Jan; Wingrove, Janet; Rimes, Katharine; Bathurst, Neil; Steiner, Herbert; Kennell–Webb, Sandra; Moulds, Michelle; Malliaris, Yanni (2007). "Rumination-focused cognitive behaviour therapy for residual depression: A case series". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 45 (9): 2144–2154. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.018. hdl:10871/11164. ISSN 0005-7967. PMID 17367751.
- ^ a b c d e "Rumination and related constructs : causes, consequences, and treatment of thinking too much | WorldCat.org". search.worldcat.org. Retrieved 2024-10-20.
- ^ Watkins, Edwards (2016). Rumination-Focused Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Depression. Guilford Publications. ISBN 9781462536047.
- ^ a b Nolen-Hoeksema S (2004). "The Response Styles Theory". In Papageorgiou C, Wells A (eds.). Depressive Rumination: Nature, Theory and Treatment. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 105–123. doi:10.1002/9780470713853.ch6. ISBN 9780470713853.
- ^ Jacobs, Rachel H.; Watkins, Edward R.; Peters, Amy T.; Feldhaus, Claudia G.; Barba, Alyssa; Carbray, Julie; Langenecker, Scott A. (23 November 2016). "Targeting Ruminative Thinking in Adolescents at Risk for Depressive Relapse: Rumination-Focused Cognitive Behavior Therapy in a Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial with Resting State fMRI". PLOS ONE. 11 (11): e0163952. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1163952J. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0163952. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 5120778. PMID 27880789.
- ^ Topper, Maurice; Emmelkamp, Paul M. G.; Watkins, Ed; Ehring, Thomas (1 March 2017). "Prevention of anxiety disorders and depression by targeting excessive worry and rumination in adolescents and young adults: A randomized controlled trial". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 90: 123–136. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2016.12.015. ISSN 0005-7967. PMID 28049069.
- ^ Perestelo-Perez, Lilisbeth; Barraca, Jorge; Peñate, Wenceslao; Rivero-Santana, Amado; Alvarez-Perez, Yolanda (September 2017). "Mindfulness-based interventions for the treatment of depressive rumination: Systematic review and meta-analysis". International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. 17 (3): 282–295. doi:10.1016/j.ijchp.2017.07.004. ISSN 1697-2600. PMC 6220915. PMID 30487903.
- ^ Borkovec TD, Robinson E, Pruzinsky T, DePree JA (1983). "Preliminary exploration of worry: some characteristics and processes". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 21 (1): 9–16. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3. PMID 6830571.
- ^ Winterman D (17 October 2013). "Rumination: The Danger of Dwelling". BBC News. Retrieved 2022-01-21.
- ^ Beck, R; Perkins, T. S. (2001). "Cognitive Content-Specificity for Anxiety and Depression: A Meta-Analysis". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 25 (6): 651–663. doi:10.1023/A:1012911104891. S2CID 30854969 – via SpringerLink.
- ^ McLaughlin KA, Borkovec TD, Sibrava NJ (March 2007). "The effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive activity". Behavior Therapy. 38 (1): 23–38. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.03.003. PMID 17292692.
- ^ Papageorgiou C, Wells A (1999). "Process and meta cognitive dimensions of depressive and anxious thoughts and relationships with emotional intensity". Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy. 5 (2): 152. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0879(199905)6:2<156::aid-cpp196>3.0.co;2-a.
- ^ Watkins E, Moulds M, Mackintosh B (December 2005). "Comparisons between rumination and worry in a non-clinical population". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 43 (12): 1577–1585. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.11.008. PMID 16239152.
- ^ Ryckman, Nathan A.; Lambert, Anthony J. (1 January 2015). "Unsuccessful suppression is associated with increased neuroticism, intrusive thoughts, and rumination". Personality and Individual Differences. 73: 88–91. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.029. ISSN 0191-8869.
- ^ a b c Rickerby, Nicole; Krug, Isabel; Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, Matthew; Forte, Elizabeth; Davenport, Rebekah; Chayadi, Ellentika; Kiropoulos, Litza (June 2024). "Rumination across depression, anxiety, and eating disorders in adults: A meta-analytic review". Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 31 (2): 251–268. doi:10.1037/cps0000110. ISSN 1468-2850.
- ^ a b c Watkins, Edward R.; Roberts, Henrietta (1 April 2020). "Reflecting on rumination: Consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of rumination". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 127: 103573. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2020.103573. ISSN 0005-7967.
Further reading
edit- Pedersen WC, Denson TF, Goss RJ, Vasquez EA, Kelley NJ, Miller N (June 2011). "The impact of rumination on aggressive thoughts, feelings, arousal, and behaviour". The British Journal of Social Psychology. 50 (Pt 2): 281–301. doi:10.1348/014466610X515696. PMID 21545459. S2CID 207265785.
- Tsai W, Chang EC, Sanna LJ, Herringshaw AJ (September 2011). "An examination of happiness as a buffer of the rumination–adjustment link: Ethnic differences between European and Asian American students". Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2 (3): 168–180. doi:10.1037/a0025319. S2CID 43930297.
- Baer RA, Sauer SE (April 2011). "Relationships between depressive rumination, anger rumination, and borderline personality features". Personality Disorders. 2 (2): 142–150. doi:10.1037/a0019478. PMID 22448733. S2CID 17006066.
- Simonson J, Mezulis A, Davis K (November 2011). "Socialized to ruminate? Gender role mediates the sex difference in rumination for interpersonal events". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 30 (9): 937–959. doi:10.1521/jscp.2011.30.9.937. S2CID 30249771.
- Dickson KS, Ciesla JA, Reilly LC (September 2012). "Rumination, worry, cognitive avoidance, and behavioral avoidance: examination of temporal effects". Behavior Therapy. 43 (3): 629–640. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.11.002. PMID 22697450.